Thứ Hai, 29 tháng 9, 2025

5 A Mind Undisturbed


 

Everything you do, be it great or small, is but one-eighth of the problem,” a sixth-century Christian monk admonished his fellow renunciates, “whereas to keep one’s state undisturbed even if thereby one should fail to accomplish the task, is the other seven-eighths.”1

 

A mind undisturbed marks a prominent goal of meditation paths in all the great spiritual traditions. Thomas Merton, a Trappist contemplative, wrote his own version of a poem lauding this very quality, taken from the ancient annals of Taoism. He tells of a craftsman who could draw perfect circles without using a compass, and whose mind was “free and without concern.”2

 

A mind unworried has as its opposite the angst life brings us: money worries, working too hard, family problems, health troubles. In nature, stress episodes like encountering a predator are temporary, giving the body time to recover. In modern life stressors are mostly psychological, not biological, and can be ongoing (if only in our thoughts), like a horrific boss or trouble with family. Such stressors trigger those same ancient biological reactions. If these stress reactions last for a long time, they can make us sick.

 

Our vulnerability to stress-worsened diseases like diabetes or hypertension reflects the downside in our brain’s design. The upside reflects the glories of the human cortex, which has built civilizations (and the computer this is being written on). But the brain’s executive center, located behind the forehead in our prefrontal cortex, gives us both a unique advantage among all animals and a paradoxical disadvantage: the ability to anticipate the future—and worry about it—as well as to think about the past —and regret.

 

As Epictetus, a Greek philosopher, put it centuries ago, it’s not the things that happen to us that are upsetting but the view we take of those doings. A more modern sentiment comes from poet Charles Bukowski: it’s not the big things that drive us mad, but “the shoelace that snaps with no time left.”

 

The science here shows that the more we perceive such hassles in our lives, the higher our levels of stress hormones like cortisol. That’s a bit ominous: cortisol, if raised chronically, has deleterious impacts like an increased risk of dying from heart disease.3 Can meditation help?

 

FROM THE BACK OF AN ENVELOPE

 

We first got to know Jon Kabat-Zinn during our Harvard days, when he had just finished his PhD in molecular biology at MIT and was exploring meditation and yoga. Jon was a student of Korean Zen master Seung Sahn, who had a meditation center in the same Cambridge neighborhood where Dan was living. And not far away, in Richie’s second-floor apartment off Harvard Square, Jon gave Richie his first instruction in meditation and yoga, shortly before Richie’s trip to India.

 

A like-minded meditating scientist, Jon had joined our team when we studied Swami X at Harvard Medical School. Jon had just gotten a fellowship in anatomy and cell biology at the newly opened University of Massachusetts Medical School in Worcester, an hour’s drive from Cambridge. The anatomy was what interested him most—Jon had already begun teaching yoga classes in Cambridge.

 

In those days Jon often went on retreats at the Insight Meditation Society (IMS), then recently founded, in Barre, also about an hour away from Boston and not far from Worcester. In 1974, several years before IMS was founded, Jon had spent two weeks one freezing early April in an unheated Girl Scout camp in the Berkshires, rented for a vipassana course. The teacher, Robert Hover, had been commissioned to teach by the Burmese master U Ba Khin, who, you might remember, was also the teacher of S. N. Goenka, whose retreats Dan and Richie attended in India.

 

Like Goenka, the main methods Hover taught were, initially, to focus on your breath in order to build concentration for the first three days of the retreat, and then to systematically scan the body’s sensations very slowly, from head to toe, over and over again for the next seven days. During the scan you focused only on the bare bodily sensations—the norm in that meditation lineage.

 

Hover’s instructions included several two-hour meditation sittings during which students vowed not to make a single voluntary movement—twice as long as those at Goenka’s courses. These immobile sessions produced a level of pain, Jon said, he had never experienced in his life. But as he sat through that unbearable pain and scanned his body to focus on his experience, the pain dissolved into pure sensations.

 

On this retreat Jon had an insight, which he quickly wrote down on the back of an envelope, that there might be a way to share the benefits of meditation practices with medical patients, especially those experiencing chronic pain that wouldn’t go away just by changing their posture or stopping the meditation practice. Coupled with a sudden vision that came to him a few years later on a retreat at IMS and that drew together disparate parts of his own practice history into a form that would be accessible to anyone, the program now known around the world as mindfulness-based stress reduction, or MBSR, came into being in September of 1979 at the University of Massachusetts Medical Center.4

 

In his vision he realized that pain clinics are filled with people whose symptoms are excruciating and who can’t escape the pain except through debilitating narcotics. He saw that the body scan and other mindfulness practices could help these patients uncouple the cognitive and emotional parts of their experience of pain from the pure sensation, a perceptual shift that can itself be a significant relief.

 

But most of these patients—a random slice of folks from the workingclass environs of Worcester—could not sit still for long periods of time like the dedicated meditators Hover taught. So Jon adapted a method from his yoga training, a lying-down body scan meditation which, similar to the Hover approach, has you connect with and then move through key regions of the body in a systematic sequence, starting with the toes of the left foot, and winding up at the top of the head. The key point: it is possible to register and then investigate and transform your relationship to whatever you are sensing at a given place in the body, even if it is highly unpleasant.

 

Borrowing from both his Zen background and vipassana, Jon added a sitting meditation where people pay careful attention to their breath, letting go of thoughts or sensations that arise—just being aware of attending itself, not of the object of attention, the breath at the beginning, and then other objects such as sounds, thoughts, emotions, and of course, bodily sensations of all kinds. And, taking another cue from Zen and vipassana, he added mindful walking, mindful eating, and a general awareness of life’s activities, including one’s relationships.

 

We were pleased that Jon pointed to our Harvard research as evidence (otherwise pretty scant in those days) that methods taken from contemplative paths and put in new forms without their spiritual context could have benefits in the modern world.5 These days that evidence has grown more than ample; MBSR has risen to the top of meditation practices undergoing scientific scrutiny. MBSR may be the most widely practiced form of mindfulness anywhere, taught around the world in hospitals and clinics, schools, even businesses. One of the many benefits claimed for MBSR: boosting how well people handle stress.

 

In an early study of the impact of MBSR on stress reactivity, Philippe Goldin (an SRI attendee) and his mentor at Stanford University, James Gross, studied a small group of patients with social anxiety disorder who underwent the standard eight-week MBSR program.6 Before and after the training, they went into the fMRI scanner, while being presented with stressors—statements taken from their own tales of social “meltdowns” and their thoughts during them—for example, “I am incompetent,” or “I am ashamed of my shyness.”

 

As these stressful thoughts were presented, the patients used either of two different attentional stances: mindful awareness of their breath or distraction by doing mental arithmetic. Only mindfulness of their breath both lowered activity in the amygdala—mainly via a faster recovery—and strengthened it in the brain’s attentional networks, while the patients reported less stress reactivity. The same beneficial pattern emerged when the patients who had done MBSR were compared with some who had trained in aerobics.7

 

That is but one of many hundreds of studies that have been done on MBSR, revealing a multitude of payoffs, as we’ll see throughout this book. But the same can be said for MBSR’s close cousin, mindfulness itself.

 

MINDFUL ATTENTION

 

When we started to participate in dialogues between the Dalai Lama and scientists at the Mind and Life Institute, we were impressed by the precision with which one of his interpreters, Alan Wallace, was able to equate scientific terms with their equivalent meanings in Tibetan, a language lacking any such technical terminology. Alan, it turned out, had a PhD in religious studies from Stanford University, extensive familiarity with quantum physics, and rigorous philosophical training, in part as a Tibetan Buddhist monk for several years.

 

Drawing on his contemplative expertise, Alan developed a unique program that extracts from the Tibetan context a meditation practice accessible to anyone, what he calls Mindful Attention Training. This program starts with full focus on the breath, then progressively refines attention to observe the natural flow of the mind stream and finally rest in the subtle awareness of awareness itself.8

 

In a study at Emory, people who had never meditated previously were randomly assigned to practice Mindful Attention Training or a compassion meditation. A third group, an active control, went through a series of discussions on health.9

 

The participants were scanned before and after they underwent eight weeks of training. While in the scanner they viewed a set of images— standard in emotion research—which includes a few upsetting ones, such as a burn victim. The Mindful Attention group showed reduced amygdala activity in response to the disturbing pictures. The changes in amygdala function occurred in the ordinary baseline state in this study, suggesting the seeds of a trait effect.

 

A word about the amygdala, which has a privileged role as the brain’s radar for threat: it receives immediate input from our senses, which it scans for safety or danger. If it perceives a threat, the amygdala circuitry triggers the brain’s freeze-fight-or-flight response, a stream of hormones like cortisol and adrenaline that mobilize us for action. The amygdala also responds to anything important to pay attention to, whether we like or dislike it.

 

The sweat dollops Dan measured in his study were distant indicators of this amygdala-driven reaction. In effect, Dan was trying to tease out a change in amygdala function—a quicker recovery from arousal—but was using a hopelessly indirect metric with the sweat response. That was in a day long before the invention of scanners that directly track activity in brain regions.

 

The amygdala connects strongly to brain circuitry for both focusing our attention and for intense emotional reactions. This dual role explains why, when we are in the grip of anxiety, we are also very distracted, especially by whatever is making us anxious. As the brain’s radar for threat, the amygdala rivets our attention on what it finds troubling. So when something worries or upsets us, our mind wanders over and over to that thing, even to the point of fixation—like the viewers of the shop accident film when they saw Al’s thumb approach that wicked saw blade.

 

About the same time as Alan’s findings that mindfulness calms the amygdala, other researchers had volunteers who had never meditated before practice mindfulness for just twenty minutes a day over one week, and then have an fMRI scan.10 During the scan they saw images ranging from gruesome burn victims to cute bunnies. They watched these images in their everyday state of mind, and then while practicing mindfulness.

 

During mindful attention their amygdala response was significantly lower (compared to nonmeditators) to all the images. This sign of being less disturbed, tellingly, was greatest in the amygdala on the brain’s right side (there are amygdalae in both right and left hemispheres), which often has a stronger response to whatever upsets us than the one on the left.

 

In this second study, lessened amygdala reactivity was found only during mindful attention and not during ordinary awareness, indicating a state effect, not an altered trait. A trait change, remember, is the “before,” not the “after.”

 

PAIN IS IN THE BRAIN

 

If you give the back of your hand a hard pinch, different brain systems mobilize, some for the pure sensation of pain and others for our dislike of that pain. The brain unifies them into a visceral, instant Ouch!

 

But that unity falls apart when we practice mindfulness of the body, spending hours noticing our bodily sensations in great detail. As we sustain this focus, our awareness morphs.

 

What had been a painful pinch transforms, breaking down into its constituents: the intensity of the pinch and the painful sensation, and the emotional feeling tone—we don’t want the pain; we urgently want the pain to stop.

 

But if we persevere with mindful investigation, that pinch becomes an experience to unpack with interest, even equanimity. We can see our aversion fall away, and the “pain” break down into subtler flavors: throbbing, heat, intensity.

 

Imagine now you hear a soft rumble as a five-gallon tank of water starts boiling and sends a stream of fluid through the thin rubber hose that runs through the two-inch square metal plate strapped tight on your wrist. The plate heats up, pleasantly at first. But that pleasantness quickly heads toward pain, as the water temperature jumps several degrees within a couple of seconds. Finally, you can’t take it anymore—if this were a hot stove you had touched, you would instantly pull away. But you can’t remove that metal plate. You feel the almost excruciating heat for a full ten seconds, sure you are getting burned.

 

But you get no burn; your skin is fine. You’ve just reached your highest pain threshold, exactly what this device, the Medoc thermal stimulator, was designed to detect. Used by neurologists to assess conditions like neuropathy that reveal deterioration of the central nervous system, the thermal stimulator has built-in safety devices so people’s skin won’t be burned, even as it calibrates precisely their maximum pain threshold. And people’s pain thresholds are nowhere near the higher range at which burns occur. That’s why the Medoc has been used with experimental volunteers to establish how meditation alters our perceptions of pain.

 

Among pain’s main components are our purely physiological sensations, like burning, and our psychological reactions to those sensations.11 Meditation, the theory goes, might mute our emotional response to pain and so make the heat sensations more bearable.

 

In Zen, for example, practitioners learn to suspend their mental reactions and categorization of whatever arises in their minds or around them, and this mental stance gradually spills over into everyday life.12 “The experienced practitioner of zazen does not depend on sitting quietly,” as Ruth Sasaki, a Zen teacher, put it, adding, “States of consciousness at first attained only in the meditation hall gradually become continuous in any and all activities.”13

 

Seasoned Zen meditators who were having their brains scanned (and who were asked to “not meditate”) endured the thermal stimulator.14 While

we’ve noted the reasons to have an active control group, this research had none. But that’s less an issue here, because of the brain imaging. If the outcome measures are based on self-reports (the most easily swayed by expectations) or even behavior observed by someone else (somewhat less susceptible to bias) then an active control group matters greatly. But when it comes to their brain activity, people have no clue what’s going on, and so an active control matters less.

 

The more experienced among the Zen students not only were able to bear more pain than could controls, they also displayed little activity in executive, evaluative, and emotion areas during the pain—all regions that ordinarily flare into activity when we are under such intense stress. Tellingly, their brains seemed to disconnect the usual link between executive center circuits where we evaluate (This hurts!) and circuitry for sensing physical pain (This burns).

 

In short, the Zen meditators seemed to respond to pain as though it was a more neutral sensation. In more technical language, their brains showed a “functional decoupling” of the higher and lower brain regions that register pain—while their sensory circuitry felt the pain, their thoughts and emotions did not react to it. This offers a new twist on a strategy sometimes used in cognitive therapy: reappraisal of severe stress—thinking about it in a less threatening way—which can lessen its subjective severity as well as the brain’s response. Here, though, the Zen meditators seemed to apply a no-appraisal neural strategy—in keeping with the mind-set of zazen itself.

 

A close reading of this article reveals a mention only in passing of a significant trait effect, in a difference found between Zen meditators and the comparison group. During the initial baseline reading the temperature is increased in a staircase-like series of finely graduated rises to calibrate the precise maximum pain threshold for each person. The Zen practitioners’ pain threshold was 2 degrees Centigrade (5.6 degrees Fahrenheit) higher than for nonmeditators.

 

This may not sound like much, but the way we experience pain from heat means that slight increases in temperature can have dramatic impact both subjectively and in how our brain responds. Though that difference of 2 degrees Centigrade may seem trivial, in the world of pain experience, it is huge.

 

Researchers are, appropriately, skeptical about such traitlike findings because self-selection in who chooses to stick with meditation and who drops out along the way might also account for such data; perhaps people who choose to meditate for years and years are already different in ways that look like trait effects. The maxim “Correlation does not mean causation” applies here.

 

But if a trait can be understood as a lasting effect of the practice, that poses an alternative explanation. And when different research groups come up with similar trait findings, these converging results make us take the result more seriously.

 

Contrast the Zen sitters’ recovery from stress reactivity with burnout, the depleted, hopeless state that comes from years of constant, unremitting pressures, like from jobs that demand too much. Burnout has become rampant among health care professions such as nurses and doctors, as well as those who care at home for loved ones with problems like Alzheimer’s. And, of course, anyone can feel burned-out who faces the rants of rude customers or continual implacable deadlines, as with the hectic pace of a business start-up.

 

Such constant stress sculpts the brain for the worse, it seems.15 Brain scans of people who for years had faced work that demanded up to seventy hours each week revealed enlarged amygdalae and weak connections between areas in the prefrontal cortex that can quiet the amygdala in a disturbing moment. And when those stressed-out workers were asked to reduce their emotional reaction to upsetting pictures, they were unable to do so—technically, a failure in “down-regulation.”

 

Like people who suffer from post-traumatic stress syndrome, victims of burnout are no longer able to put a halt to their brain’s stress response—and so, never have the healing balm of recovery time.

 

There are tantalizing results that indirectly support meditation’s role in resilience. A collaboration between Richie’s lab and the research group directed by Carol Ryff looked at a subset of participants in a large, multisite, national study of midlife in the United States. They found that the stronger a person’s sense of purpose in life, the more quickly they recovered from a lab stressor.16

 

Having a sense of purpose and meaning may let people meet life’s challenges better, reframing them in ways that allow them to recover more readily. And, as we saw in chapter three, meditation seems to enhance wellbeing on Ryff’s measure, which includes a person’s sense of purpose. So what’s the direct evidence that meditation can help us meet upsets and challenges with more aplomb?

 

BEYOND CORRELATION

 

When Dan taught the psychology of consciousness course in 1975 at Harvard, Richie, then in his last year of graduate school, was, as mentioned, a teaching assistant. Among the students he met with weekly was Cliff Saron, then a senior at Harvard. Cliff had a knack for the technical end of research, including the electronics (perhaps a legacy of his father, Bob Saron, who had managed the sound equipment at NBC). Cliff’s adeptness soon made him a coauthor on research papers with Richie.

 

And when Richie got his first teaching post at the State University of New York at Purchase, he took Cliff along to manage the laboratory. After a stint there—and coauthoring a slew of scientific papers with Richie—Cliff got his own PhD in neuroscience at Albert Einstein College of Medicine. He now directs a lab at the Center for Mind and Brain at the University of California at Davis, and has often been on the faculty at the Mind and Life Summer Research Institute.

 

Cliff’s astute sense of methodological issues no doubt helped him design and run a crucial bit of research, one of the few longitudinal studies of meditation to date.17 With Alan Wallace as retreat leader, Cliff put together a rigorous battery of assessments for students going through a three-month training in a range of classic meditation styles, including some, like mindfulness of breathing, meant to increase focus and others to cultivate positive states like loving-kindness and equanimity. While the “yogis” pursued their demanding schedule of meditating six or more hours a day for ninety days, Cliff had them take a battery of tests at the beginning, middle, and end of the retreat, and five months after the retreat had concluded.18

 

 The comparison group was people who had signed up for the threemonth retreat but who did not start until the first group finished. Such a “wait-list” control eliminates worries about expectation demand and similar psychological confounds (but does not add an active control like HEP— which would be a logistic and financial burden in a study like this). A stickler for precision in research, Cliff flew people in the wait-list group to the retreat place and gave them exactly the same assessments in the identical context as those in the retreat.

 

One test presented lines of different lengths in rapid succession, with the instruction to press one button for a line that was shorter than the others. Only one out of ten lines was short; the challenge is to inhibit the knee-jerk tendency to press the button for a short line when a long one appears. As the retreat progressed, so did the ability of the meditators to control this impulse—a mirror on a skill critical to managing our emotion, the capacity to refrain from acting on whim or impulse.

 

This simple skill, statistical analyses suggested, led to a range of improvements on self-reports, from less anxiety to an overall sense of wellbeing, including emotion regulation as gauged by reports of recovering more quickly from upsets and more freedom from impulses. Tellingly, the wait-list controls showed no change in any of these measures—but showed the same improvements once they had gone through the retreat.

 

Cliff’s study directly ties these benefits to meditation, lending strong support to the case for altered traits. A clincher: a follow-up five months after the retreats ended found that the improvements remained.

 

And the study dispels doubts that all the positive traits found in long-term meditators are simply due to self-selection, where people who already had those traits choose the practice or stay with it in the long run. From evidence like this, it seems likely that the states we practice in meditation gradually spill over into daily life to mold our traits—at least when it comes to handling stress.

 

A DEVILISH ORDEAL

 

Imagine you are describing your qualifications for a job while two interviewers glare at you, unsmiling. Their faces reveal no empathy, not even an encouraging nod. That’s the situation in the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), one of the most reliable ways known to science to trigger the brain’s stress circuits and its cascade of stress hormones.

 

Now imagine, after that dispiriting job interview, doing some pressured mental arithmetic: you have to subtract 13s in rapid-fire succession from a number like 1,232. That’s the second part of the Trier test, and those same impassive interviewers push you to do the math faster and faster—and whenever you make a mistake, they tell you to start all over at 1,232. That devilish test delivers a huge dose of social stress, the awful feelings we get when other people evaluate, reject, or exclude us.

 

Alan Wallace and Paul Ekman created a renewal program for schoolteachers that combined psychological training with meditation.19 Whereas Dan had used the shop accident film to bring stress into the lab, here the stressor was the Trier test’s simulated job interview followed by that formidable math challenge.

 

The more hours those teachers had practiced meditation, the quicker their blood pressure recovered from a high point during the TSST. This was true five months after the program ended, suggesting at least a mild trait effect (five years afterward would be still stronger evidence of a trait).

 

Richie’s lab used the Trier with seasoned (lifetime average = 9,000 hours) vipassana meditators who did an eight-hour day of meditation and the next day underwent the test.20 The meditators and their age- and gendermatched comparison group all went through the TSST (as well as a test for inflammation—more on those results in chapter nine, “Mind, Body, and Genome”).

 

Result: the meditators had a smaller rise in cortisol during the stress. Just as important, the meditators perceived that dreaded Trier test as less stressful than did the nonmeditators.

 

This cooled-out, more balanced way of viewing that stressor among the seasoned meditators was not tapped while they were practicing but while they were at rest—our “before.” Their ease during both the stressful interview and the formidable mental math challenge seems a genuine trait effect.

 

Further evidence for this comes from research with these same advanced meditators.21 The meditators’ brains were scanned while they saw disturbing images of people suffering, like burn victims. The seasoned practitioners’ brains revealed a lowered level of reactivity in the amygdala; they were more immune to emotional hijacking.

 

The reason: their brains had stronger operative connectivity between the prefrontal cortex, which manages reactivity, and the amygdala, which triggers such reactions. As neuroscientists know, the stronger this particular link in the brain, the less a person will be hijacked by emotional downs and ups of all sorts.

 

This connectivity modulates a person’s level of emotional reactivity: the stronger the link, the less reactive. Indeed, that relationship is so strong that a person’s reactivity level can be predicted by the connectivity. So, when these high-lifetime-hour meditators saw an image of a gruesome-looking burn victim, they had little amygdala reactivity. Age-matched volunteers did not show either the heightened connectivity or the equanimity on viewing the disturbing images.

 

But when Richie’s group repeated this study with people taking the MBSR training (a total of just under thirty hours) plus a bit of daily at-home practice, they failed to find any strengthening of connection between the prefrontal region and the amygdala during the challenge of upsetting images. Nor was there any when the MBSR group simply rested.

 

While MBSR training did reduce the reactivity of the amygdala, the long-term meditator group showed both this reduced reactivity in the amygdala plus strengthening of the connection between the prefrontal cortex and amygdala. This pattern implies that when the going gets tough— for example, in response to a major life challenge such as losing a job—the ability to manage distress (which depends upon the connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and amygdala) will be greater in long-term meditators compared to those who have only done the MBSR training.

 

The good news is that this resilience can be learned. What we don’t know is how long this effect might last. We suspect that it would be short-lived unless participants continued to practice, a key to transforming a state into a trait.

 

Among those who show the most short-lived amygdala response, emotions come and go, adaptive and appropriate. Richie’s lab put this idea to the test with brain scans of 31 highly seasoned meditators (lifetime average was 8,800 hours of meditation practice, ranging from just 1,200 to more than 30,000).

 

They saw the usual pictures ranging from people in extreme suffering (burn victims) to cute bunnies. On first analysis of the expert meditators’ amygdalae, there was no difference in how they reacted from the responses of matched volunteers who had never meditated. But when Richie’s group divided the seasoned meditators into those with the least hours of practice (lifetime average 1,849 hours) and the most (lifetime average 7,118), the results showed that the more hours of practice, the more quickly the amygdala recovered from distress.22

 

This rapid recovery is the hallmark of resilience. In short, equanimity emerges more strongly with extended practice. Among the benefits of longterm meditation, this tells us, are exactly what those Desert Fathers were after: a mind undisturbed.

 

IN A NUTSHELL

 

The amygdala, a key node in the brain’s stress circuitry, shows dampened activity from a mere thirty or so hours of MBSR practice. Other mindfulness training shows a similar benefit, and there are hints in the research that these changes are traitlike: they appear not simply during the explicit instruction to perceive the stressful stimuli mindfully but even in the “baseline” state, with reductions in amygdala activation as great as 50 percent. Such lessening of the brain’s stress reactions appears in response not simply to seeing the gory pictures used in the laboratory but also to more real-life challenges like the stressful Trier interview before a live audience. More daily practice seems associated with lessened stress reactivity. Experienced Zen practitioners can withstand higher levels of pain, and have less reaction to this stressor. A three-month meditation retreat brought indicators of better emotional regulation, and long-term practice was associated with greater functional connectivity between the prefrontal areas that manage emotion and the areas of the amygdala that react to stress, resulting in less reactivity. And an improved ability to regulate attention accompanies some of the beneficial impact of meditation on stress reactivity. Finally, the quickness with which long-term meditators recover from stress underlines how trait effects emerge with continued practice./.

6

Thứ Sáu, 19 tháng 9, 2025

TỊNH ĐỘ TÔNG TỪ BÀI 1 ĐẾN BÀI 15

 



01-Bài hướng nguyện A Di Đà

02-Bài Phát Nguyện Vãng Sinh Cực Lạc - Đại Sư Tông Phật Khách Ba -

03-Cực lạc và luân hồi: Bất nhị trong tịnh độ tông.

04-Giáo Pháp của Phật Di Đà trong thế giới hiện đại – 11/01/2011.

05-Hoàn tướng hồi hướng - Tác giả: J. Paraskevopoulos.

06-Long Thọ Với Phật Di Đà và Cõi Tịnh Độ.

07-Phát nguyện vãng sinh Cực lạc.-  Larry Cappel 

08-Thiện ác là gì?Tác giả: Yoshifumi Ueda, Chuyển ngữ: Tuệ Uyển

09-Thiên Thân Tịnh Độ Luận, Nguyên tác: Vasubandhu.

10-Thiền trong Tịnh độ tông - Tiến sĩ Alfred Bloom.

11-Tịnh độ chân tông. -  Rev. Shojo Honda

12-Tịnh độ chân tông thực hành.

13-Tinh hoa của Đại thừa là quan điểm “hồi nhập ta bà” - 15/01/2011.

14-Vọng tưởng luân hồi.Tác giả: Agnes Jedrzejewska 

15-Quy Mạng Phật Di Đà Và Cực Lạc- Long Thọ Đại Sĩ

ĐỨC ĐẠT LAI LẠT MA TỪ BÀI 1 ĐẾN BÀI 15

 



 

 01-Âm Nhạc Và  Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma.

02-Bên lề hào nhoáng - Tác giả:Ron Gluckman.

03-Đạt Lai Lạt Ma, Con Trai Tôi, Diki Tsering.

04-Đạt Lai Lạt Ma, Thúc Phụ Của Tôi - Khedroop Thondup.

05-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma phát biểu tại lễ nhận giải Templeton.

06-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma và Giải Nobel Hòa bình.

07-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma và Huân chương vàng quốc hội Hoa Kỳ.

08-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma thứ 14 và Tây Tạng - Tuệ Uyển

09-Hành trình tâm linh của tôi - Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma.

11-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Và Mười Câu Hỏi.

12-Tay Trong Tay Với Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma - Perry Garfinkel.

13-Thông Điệp của Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Gởi Tới Đồng Bào Tây Tạng Nhân Dịp Tân Niên Năm Con Trâu Đất.

14-Tuyên bố về việc nhà bất đồng chính kiến Trung Quốc Lưu Hiểu Ba được trao giải Nobel Hòa Bình 2010.

15-Lời cảm ơn của Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma nhân sinh nhật thứ 82



PHỎNG VẤN ĐỨC ĐẠT LAI LẠT MA TỪ BÀI 1 ĐẾN BÀI 20

01-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Đàm Luận Với Đài Abc.

02-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma đàm luận với Hoàng tử Panu của Thái Lan.

03-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Đàm Luận Với Maureen Cavanaugh.

04-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma đàm luận với một nhóm Phật tử Đông Nam Á.

05-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Đàm Luận Với Phật Tử Thái Lan.

06-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma đàm luận với Robert Thurman.

07-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma đàm luận với tạp chí Newsweek.

08-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Đàm Luận Với Tạp Chí Phòng Vệ Và Cảnh Báo An Ninh.

09-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma đàm luận với tạp chí Rolling Stones - 02/08/2011.

10-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma đàm luận với tạp chí Time - 05/08/2011.

11-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma nói chuyện với thiếu niên 11 tuổi.

12-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma trả lời những câu hỏi từ Hoa Lục.

13-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Trả Lời Phỏng Vấn Của Wang Lixiong - Tuệ Uyển chuyển ngữ.

14-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma Trao Đổi Với Người Trung Quốc Qua Mạng Twitter.

15-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma vấn đáp với Glassman.

16-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma vấn đáp với Oprah.

17-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma vấn đáp với Raimondo.

18-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma vấn đáp với Spalding Gray.

19-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma vấn đáp với tạp chí Trung Hoa ngày nay.

20-Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma: Vấn Đáp Với Đài Voa.



PHẬT GIÁO &CÁC TÔN GIÁO TỪ BẢI 1 ĐẾN BÀI 10

 


01-Nhiều Niềm Tin, Một Chân Lý -Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma 

02-Thách Thức Của Nhân Loại: Phát Biểu Về Liên Tôn - Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma

03-Có Phải Chúa Giê-Su Đã Đến Ấn Độ Để Học Phật Pháp - By Madhusree Chatterjee -

04-Đạo Phật với các tôn giáo và khoa học - Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma 

05-Hòa hiệp từ bi tôn giáo và Hồi giáo - H H. the Fourteenth Dalai Lama. 

06-Nhiều Niềm Tin, Một Chân Lý - Tenzin Gyatso (Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma).

07-Những Không Gian Tâm Linh - HH. the Dalai Lama.

08-Quan điểm của Phật Giáo đối với những tôn giáo khác. - Berzin Alexander and Chodron, Thubten.

09-Sự Hợp Tác Giữa Những Tôn Giáo Thế Giới - HH the Dalai Lama. 

10-Thách Thức Của Nhân Loại: Phát Biểu Về Liên Tôn - Đức Đạt Lai Lạt Ma

TRÍ TUỆ NHÂN TẠO “AI” TỪ BÀI 1 ĐẾN BÀI 40


01- “Ai” Nói Gì Về Phật Tử Sẽ Nói Về Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo Phổ Quát

02- “Ai” Có Liên Quan Gì Đến Phật Giáo?

03-“AI” Sự Hội Tụ Của Công Nghệ Và Tâm Linh

04- Ai Trao Sức Mạnh Mềm Cho Phật Giáo

05- Ảnh Hưởng Của Phật Giáo Đến “Ai” Và Kiểm Soát Khí Hậu 

06-Công Cụ Ai Buddha Này Sẽ Cho Bạn Biết Về Con Đường Dễ Dàng Để Giác Ngộ

07- CHATGPT + Phật Giáo Tham Gia Xã Hội, Phần I

08- CHATGPT + Phật Giáo Tham Gia Xã Hội, Phần II

09- Giáo Lý, Kiến Thức Hoàn Hảo Và Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo

10- ĐỊnh Nghĩa Về Ý Thức: Phật Giáo Có Thể Truyền Đạt Thông Tin Cho Ai Như Thế Nào

11- Giữa Nhân Tạo Và Con Người

12- Khủng Hỏang Siêu Hình: Phật Giáo Và “AI”

13- Liệu Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo (Ai) Có Đạt Được Khả Năng Tri Giác Không?

14- Lời Ca Ngợi Cho Một Bài Thánh Thi Hopepunk

15-Lương Tri Đạo Đức Phật Giáo Và Những Hệ Quả Của Sự Thông Minh Nhân Tạo

16-Máy Móc Có Thể Là Con Người Không? You 2 Và Quan Điểm Phật Giáo Về Bản Thân

17-Phật Giáo Có Thể Dạy Chúng Ta Điều Gì Về Sự Thông Minh Nhân Tạo Và Về Chính Chúng Ta

18-Phật Giáo: Con Đường Đến Giác Ngộ — Con Đường Vượt Qua Trí Tuệ

19- Phật Môn Viễn Cảnh: Câu Đố Phật Giáo Của “AI”

20- Phật Tử Mong Muốn Xây Dựng Quan Hệ Đối Tác Toàn Cầu Nhưng Thận Trọng Khi Phát Động Phong Trào Hòa Bình

21-Quan Điểm Của Pht Giáo Về Ai: Nuôi Dưỡng Sự Tự Do Quan Tâm Và Sự Đa Dạng Thật Sự Trong Tương Lai Của Ai

22- Sự Chuyển Hóa Của Phật Giáo Trong Thời Đại Kỹ Thuật Số: Ai (Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo) Và Phật Giáo Nhân Gian

23- Sự Trỗi Dậy Của Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo Và Ý Nghĩa Của Nó Đối Với Công Việc Của Chúng Ta

24- Tình Cảm Chân Thành Dành Cho Sự Nhân Tạo? 

25- Thách Thức Của Việc Sử Dụng Công Nghệ Kỹ Thuật Số Một Cách Có Chánh Niệm – Quan Điểm Của PhậT Giáo

26- *Thâm Nhập* Bồ Tát Kỷ Thuật Số _/\_

27- Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo Có Phật Tính Không?

28- Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo Và Giáo Pháp : Những Cân Nhắc Thêm

29- Trí Tuệ Phật Giáo Cho “Ai” Ít Nhân Tạo Và Thông Minh Hơn

30- Trong Một Thế Giới Của Sự Thiếu Hiểu Biết Của Con Người, Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo Có Thế Giúp Ích Không?

31- Ý Thức, Sự Chú Ý Và Công Nghệ Thông Minh: Một Bước Ngoặt Nghiệp Quả?

32-Trí Tuệ Nhân Tạo Như Một Công Cụ Cho Hòa Bình

33-Ý Nghĩa Của “Ai” Đối Với Pht Giáo

34-Nguyên Lý Phật Giáo Có Thể Giúp Định Hình Ai Đạo Đức Hơn Như Thế Nào

35-Xây Dựng Bồ Tát: Hướng Tới Một Mô Hình Trí Thông Minh Mạnh Mẽ, Đáng Tin Cậy Và Chu Đáo

36-Nguyên Tử Của Một Suy Nghĩ: Trí Tuệ Cảm Xúc Trong Kỷ Nguyên “Ai”

37-Những Suy Ngẫm Sâu Hơn Về Công Nghệ Và Giáo Lý Pht Giáo

38-Sự Gián Đoạn Từ Các Công Nghệ Sáng Tạo

39-Đức Phật Có Xuất Hiện Trên Facebook Không?

40-Không Gian Luân Hồi