Dan’s
second stay in Asia was in 1973, this time on a Social Science Research Council
postdoc, ostensibly a venture in “ethnopsychology,” to study Asian systems for
analyzing the mind and its possibilities. It started with six months in Kandy,
a town in the hills of Sri Lanka where Dan consulted every few days with
Nyanaponika Thera, a German-born Theravadan monk whose scholarship centered on
the theory and practice of meditation. (Dan then continued on for several
months in Dharamsala, India, where he studied at the Library of Tibetan Works
and Archives.)
Nyanaponika’s
writings focused on the Abhidhamma, a model of mind that laid out a map and
methods for the transformation of consciousness in the direction of altered
traits. While the Visuddhimagga and the meditation manuals Dan had read were
operator’s instructions for the mind, the Abhidhamma was a guiding theory for
such manuals. This psychological system came with a detailed explanation of the
mind’s key elements and how to traverse this inner landscape to make lasting
changes in our core being.
Certain
sections were compelling in their relevance to psychology, particularly the
dynamic outlined between “healthy” and “unhealthy” states of mind.1 All too
often our mental states fluctuate in a range that highlights desires,
self-centeredness, sluggishness, agitation, and the like. These are among the
unhealthy states on this map of mind.
Healthy
states, in contrast, include even-mindedness, composure, ongoing mindfulness,
and realistic confidence. Intriguingly, a subset of healthy states applies to
both mind and body: buoyancy, flexibility, adaptability, and pliancy.
The
healthy states inhibit the unhealthy ones, and vice versa. The mark of progress
along this path is whether our reactions in daily life signal a shift toward
healthy states. The goal is to establish the healthy states as predominant,
lasting traits.
While
immersed in deep concentration, a meditator’s unhealthy states are
suppressed—but, as with that yogi in the bazaar, can emerge as strong as ever
when the concentrative state subsides. In contrast, according to this ancient
Buddhist psychology, attaining deepening levels of insight practice leads to a
radical transformation, ultimately freeing the meditator’s mind of the
unhealthy mix. A highly advanced practitioner effortlessly stabilizes on the
healthy side, embodying confidence, buoyancy, and the like.
Dan
saw this Asian psychology as a working model of the mind, timetested over the
course of centuries, a theory of how mental training could lead to highly
positive altered traits. That theory had guided meditation practice for more
than two millennia—it was an electrifying proof of concept.
In
the summer of 1973, Richie and Susan came to Kandy for a six-week visit before
heading to India for that thrilling and sobering retreat with Goenka. Once
together in Kandy, Richie and Dan trekked through the jungle to consult with
Nyanaponika at his remote hermitage about this model of mental well-being.2
Later that year, after Dan returned from this
second sojourn in Asia as a Social Science Research Fellow, he was hired at
Harvard as a visiting lecturer. In the fall semester of 1974 he offered a
course, The Psychology of Consciousness, which fit well the ethos of those
days—at least among students, many of whom were doing their own extracurricular
research with psychedelics, yoga, and even a bit of meditation.
Once
the psychology of consciousness course was announced, hundreds of Harvard
undergrads gravitated to this survey of meditation and its altered states, the
Buddhist psychological system, and what little was then known about the
dynamics of attention—all among the topics covered. The enrollment was so large
that the class was moved into the largest classroom venue at Harvard, the 1,000-seat
Sanders Theatre.3 Richie, then in his third year of graduate school, was a
teaching assistant in the course.4
Most
of the topics in The Psychology of Consciousness—and the course title
itself—were far outside the conventional map of psychology in those days. No
surprise, Dan was not asked to stay on by the department after that semester
finished. But by then we had done some writing and research together, and
Richie was excited by the realization that this was what his own research path
would be and was eager to get going.
Starting
while we were in Sri Lanka and continuing during Dan’s semester teaching that
course on the psychology of consciousness, we worked on the first draft of our
article, making the case to our colleagues in psychology for altered traits.
While Dan had, of necessity, based his first article on thin claims, scant
research, and much guesswork, now we had a template for the path to altered
traits, an algorithm for inner transformation. We wrestled with how to connect
this map with the sparse data science had by then yielded.
Back
in Cambridge we mulled all this over in long conversations, often in Harvard
Square. As vegetarians at the time, we settled on caramel sundaes at Bailey’s
ice cream parlor on Brattle Street. There we worked on what would become a
journal article piecing together the little relevant data we could find to
support our first statement of extremely positive altered traits.
We
called it “The Role of Attention in Meditation and Hypnosis: A Psychobiological
Perspective on Transformations of Consciousness.” The operative phrase here is
transformations of consciousness, our term then for altered traits, which we
saw as a “psychobiological” (today we’d say “neural”) shift. We contended that
hypnosis, unlike meditation, produced primarily state effects, and not trait
effects as with meditation.
In
those times the fascination was not with traits but rather altered states,
whether from psychedelics or meditation. But, as we put it in talking at
Bailey’s, “after the high goes, you’re still the same schmuck you were before.”
We articulated the idea more formally in the subsequent journal article.
We
were speaking to a basic confusion, still too common, about how meditation can
change us. Some people fixate on the remarkable states attained during a
meditation session—particularly during long retreats—and give little notice to
how, or even if, those states translate into a lasting change for the better in
their qualities of being after they’ve gone home. Valuing just the heights
misses the true point of practice: to transform ourselves in lasting ways day
to day.
More
recently, this point was driven home to us when we had the chance to tell the
Dalai Lama about the meditative states and their brain patterns that a longtime
practitioner displayed in Richie’s lab. As this expert engaged in specific kinds
of meditation—for instance, concentration or visualization—the brain imaging
data revealed a distinct neural profile for each meditative altered state.
“It’s
very good,” the Dalai Lama commented, “he managed to show some signs of yogic
ability”—by which he meant the intensive meditation over months or years
practiced by yogis in Himalayan caves, as opposed to the garden variety of yoga
for fitness so popular these days.5
But
then he added, “The true mark of a meditator is that he has disciplined his mind
by freeing it from negative emotions.”
That
rule of thumb has stayed constant since before the time of the Visuddhimagga:
It’s not the highs along the way that matter. It’s who you become.
Puzzling
over how to reconcile the meditation map with what we had experienced
ourselves, and then with the admittedly scant scientific evidence, we
articulated a hypothesis: The after is
the before for the next during.
To
unpack this idea, after refers to enduring changes from meditation that last
long beyond the practice session itself. Before means the condition we are in
at baseline, before we start meditating. During is what happens as we meditate,
temporary changes in our state that pass when we stop meditating.
In
other words, repeated practice of meditation results in lasting traits— the after.
We
were intrigued by the possibility of some biological pathway where repeated
practice led to a steady embodiment of highly positive traits like kindness,
patience, presence, and ease under any circumstances. Meditation, we argued,
was a tool to foster precisely such beneficial fixtures of being.
We
published our article in one of maybe two or three academic publications
interested in such exotic topics as meditation back in the 1970s.6 This was a
first glimmer of our thinking on altered traits, albeit with a flimsy science
base. The maxim “probability is not proof” applied, in a sense: what we had was
a possibility, but little to pin a probability on, and zero proof.
When
we first wrote about this, no scientific study had been conducted that would
provide the kind of evidence we needed. Only long decades after we published
the article would Richie find that for highly adept meditators, their “before”
state was, indeed, very different from that of people who had never meditated,
or done very little meditating—it was an indicator of an altered trait (as
we’ll see in chapter twelve, “Hidden Treasure”).
No
one in psychology in those days had talked about altered traits. Plus, our raw
material was highly unusual for psychologists: ancient meditation manuals, then
hard to come by outside Asia, along with our own experiences in intensive
meditation retreats, and chance meetings with highly adept practitioners. We
were, to say the least, outliers in psychology —or oddballs, as we no doubt
were perceived by some of our Harvard colleagues.
Our
vision of altered traits made a leap far beyond the psychological science of
our day. Risky business.
THE
SCIENCE CATCHES UP
When
an imaginative researcher concocts a novel idea, it starts a chain of events
much like natural variation in evolution: as sound empirical tests weigh new
ideas, they eliminate bad hypotheses and spread good ones.7
For
this to happen, science needs to balance skeptics with speculators— people who
cast wide nets, think imaginatively, and consider “what if.” The web of
knowledge grows by testing original ideas brought to it by speculators like
ourselves. If only skeptics pursued science, little innovation would occur.
Economist
Joseph Schumpeter has become known these days for the concept of “creative
destruction,” where the new disrupts the old in a market. Our early hunches
about altered traits fit what Schumpeter called “vision”: an intuitive act that
supplies direction and energy for analytic efforts. A vision lets you see
things in a new light, as he says, one “not to be found in the facts, methods,
and results of the preexisting state of the science.”8
Sure,
we had a vision in this sense—but we had paltry methods or data available for
exploring this positive range of altered traits, and no idea of the brain
mechanism that would allow such a profound shift. We were determined to make
the argument, but were years too soon for the crucial scientific piece in this
puzzle.
Our
dissertation data were feebly—very feebly—supportive of the idea that the more
you practice how to generate a meditative state, the more that practice shows
lasting influences beyond the session itself.
Still,
as brain science has evolved over the decades, we saw mounting rationales for
our ideas.
Richie
attended his first meeting of the Society for Neuroscience in 1975 in New York
City, along with about 2,500 other scientists, all exhilarated that they were
seeing the birth of a new field (and none dreaming that these days those
meetings would draw more than 30,000 neuroscientists).9 In the mid-1980s one of
the early presidents of the society, Bruce McEwen of Rockefeller University,
gave us scientific ammunition.
McEwen
put a dominant tree shrew in the same cage for twenty-eight days with one lower
in the pecking order—the rodent version of being trapped at work with a
nightmare boss 24/7 for a month. The big shock from McEwen’s study was that in
the brain of the dominated rodent, dendrites shrank in the hippocampus, a node
crucial for memory. These branching projections of the body’s cells allow them
to reach out to and act on other cells; shrinking dendrites mean faulty memory.
McEwen’s results ripped through the brain and
behavioral sciences like a small tsunami, opening minds to the possibility that
a given experience could leave an imprint on the brain. McEwen was zeroing in
on a holy grail for psychology: how stressful events produce lingering neural
scars. That an experience of any kind could leave its mark on the brain had,
until then, been unthinkable.
To
be sure, stress was par for the course for a laboratory rat—McEwen just upped
the intensity. The standard setup for lab rat living quarters was the rodent
equivalent of solitary confinement: weeks or months on end in a small wire cage
and, if the rat was lucky, a running wheel for exercise.
Contrast
that life in perpetual boredom and social isolation to something like a rodent
health resort, with lots of toys, things to climb on, colorful walls,
playmates, and interesting spaces to explore. That’s the stimulating habitat
Marion Diamond at the University of California at Berkeley built for her lab
rats. Working about the same time as McEwen, Diamond found the rats’ brains
benefited, with thicker dendritic branches connecting neurons and growth in
brain areas, such as the prefrontal cortex, that are crucial in attention and
self-regulation.10
While
McEwen’s work showed how adverse events can shrink parts of the brain,
Diamond’s emphasized the positive in her studies. Yet her work was largely met
with a shrug in neuroscience, perhaps because it posed a direct challenge to
pervasive beliefs in the field. The conventional wisdom then was that at birth
we host in our skull a maximum number of neurons, and then inexorably lose them
in a steady die-off over the course of life. Experience, supposedly, had
nothing to do with this.
But
McEwen and Diamond led us to wonder, If these brain changes for worse and for
better could occur with rats, might the right experience change the human brain
toward beneficial altered traits? Could meditation be just such a helpful inner
workout?
The
glimpse of this possibility was exhilarating. We sensed something truly
revolutionary was in the offing, but it took a couple more decades before the
evidence began to catch up with our hunch.
THE
BIG LEAP
The
year was 1992, and Richie was nervous when the sociology department at the
University of Wisconsin asked him to deliver a major departmental colloquium.
He knew he was walking into the center of an intellectual cyclone, a battle
over “nature” and “nurture” that had raged for years in the social sciences.
The nurture camp believed that our behavior was shaped by our experiences; the
“nature” camp saw our genes as determining our behavior.
The
battle had a long, ugly history—racists in the nineteenth and early twentieth
centuries twisted the genetics of their day as “scientific” grounds for bias
against blacks, Native Americans, Jews, the Irish, and a long list of other
targets of bigotry. The racists attributed any and all lags in educational and
economic attainments of the target group to their genetic destiny, ignoring
vast imbalances in opportunity. The resulting backlash in the social sciences
had made many in that sociology department deeply skeptical of any biological
explanation.
But
Richie felt that sociologists committed a scientific fallacy in immediately
assuming that biological causes necessarily reduced group differences to
genetics—and so were seen as unchangeable. In Richie’s view, these sociologists
were carried away by an ideological stance.
For
the first time in public he proposed the concept of “neuroplasticity” as a way
to resolve this battle between nature and nurture. Neuroplasticity, he
explained, shows that repeated experience can change the brain, shaping it. We
don’t have to choose between nature or nurture. They interact, each molding the
other.
The
concept neatly reconciled what had been hostile points of view. But Richie was
reaching beyond the science of the day; the data on human neuroplasticity were
still hazy.
That
changed just a few years later with a cascade of scientific findings —for
instance, those showing that mastering a musical instrument enlarged the
relevant brain centers.11 Violinists, whose left hands continuously fingered
the strings while they played, had enlarged areas of the brain that manage that
finger work. The longer they had played, the greater the size.12
NATURE’S
EXPERIMENT
Try
this. Look straight ahead and hold up a finger with your arm outstretched.
Still looking straight ahead, slowly shift that finger until it is about two
feet to the right of your nose. When you move your finger far to the right, but
stay focused straight ahead, it lands in your peripheral vision, the outer edge
of what your visual system takes in.13
Most
people lose sight of their finger as it moves to the far right or left of their
nose. But one group does not: people who are deaf.
While
this unusual visual advantage in the deaf has long been known, the brain basis
has only recently been shown. And the mechanism is, again, neuroplasticity.
Brain studies like this take advantage of
so-called “experiments of nature,” naturally occurring situations such as
congenital deafness. Helen Neville, a neuroscientist at the University of
Oregon with a passionate interest in brain plasticity, seized the opportunity
to use an MRI brain scanner to test both deaf and hearing people with a visual
simulation that mimicked what a deaf person sees when reading sign language.
Signs
are expansive gestures. When a deaf person is reading the signing of another,
she typically looks at the face of the person who is signing—not directly at
how the hands move as they sign. Some of those expansive gestures move in the
periphery of the visual field, and thus naturally exercise the brain’s ability
to perceive within this outer rim of vision. Plasticity lets these circuits
take on a visual task as the deaf person learns sign language: reading what’s
going on at the very edge of vision.
The
chunk of neural real estate that usually operates as the primary auditory
cortex (known as Heschl’s gyrus) receives no sensory inputs in deaf people. The
brains of deaf people, Neville discovered, had morphed so that what is
ordinarily a part of the auditory system was now working with the visual
circuitry.14
Such
findings illustrate how radically the brain can rewire itself in response to
repeated experiences.15 The findings in musicians and in the deaf—and a slew of
others—offered a proof we had been waiting for. Neuroplasticity provides an
evidence-based framework and a language that makes sense in terms of current
scientific thinking.16 It was the scientific platform we had long needed, a way
of thinking about how intentional training of the mind, like meditation, might
shape the brain.
THE
ALTERED TRAIT SPECTRUM
Altered
traits map along a spectrum starting at the negative end, with posttraumatic
stress disorder (PTSD) as a case in point. The amygdala acts as the neural
radar for threat. Overwhelming trauma resets to a hair trigger the amygdala’s
threshold for hijacking the rest of the brain to respond to what it perceives
as an emergency.17 In people with PTSD, any cue that reminds them of the
traumatic experience—and that for someone else would not be particularly
noticeable—sets off a cascade of neural overreactions that create the
flashbacks, sleeplessness, irritability, and hypervigilant anxiety of that
disorder.
Moving
along the trait spectrum toward the positive range, there are the beneficial
neural impacts of being a secure child, whose brain gets molded by empathic,
concerned, and nurturing parenting. This childhood brain shaping builds in
adulthood, for example, into being able to calm down well when upset.18
Chuyenngui
Our
interest in altered traits looks beyond the merely healthy spectrum to an even
more beneficial range, wholesome traits of being. These extremely positive
altered traits, like equanimity and compassion, are a goal of mind training in
contemplative traditions. We use the term altered trait as shorthand for this
highly positive range.19
Neuroplasticity
offers a scientific basis for how repeated training could create those lasting
qualities of being we had encountered in a handful of exceptional yogis,
swamis, monks, and lamas. Their altered traits fit ancient descriptions of
lasting transformation at the higher levels.
A
mind free from disturbance has value in lessening human suffering, a goal
shared by science and meditative paths alike. But apart from lofty heights of
being, there’s a more practical potential within reach of every one of us: a
life best described as flourishing.
FLOURISHING
As
Alexander the Great was leading his armies through what is now Kashmir, legend
has it he met a group of ascetic yogis in Taxila, then a thriving city on a branch
of the Silk Road leading to the plains of India.
The
yogis responded to the appearance of Alexander’s fierce soldiers with
indifference, saying that he, like them, could actually possess only the ground
on which he stood—and that he, like them, would die one day.
The
Greek-derived word for these yogis is gymnosophists, literally “naked
philosophers” (even today some groups of Indian yogis roam naked, coating
themselves in ashes). Alexander, impressed by their equanimity, deemed them to
be “free men,” and even convinced one yogi, Kalyana, to accompany him on his
journey of conquest. No doubt the yogi’s lifestyle and outlook resonated with
Alexander’s own schooling. Alexander had been tutored by the Greek philosopher
Aristotle. Renowned for his lifelong love of learning, Alexander would have
recognized the yogis as exemplars of another source of wisdom.
The
Greek schools of philosophy espoused an ideal of personal transformation that
remarkably echoes those of Asia, as Alexander may have found in his exchanges
with Kalyana. The Greeks and their heirs the Romans, of course, laid the
foundation for Western thought down to the present day.
Aristotle
posited the goal of life as a virtue-based eudaimonia—a quality of
flourishing—a view that continues under many guises in modern thought. Virtues,
Aristotle said, are attained in part by finding the “right mean” between
extremes; courage lies between impulsive risk-taking and cowardice, a tempered
moderation between self-indulgence and ascetic denial.
And,
he added, we are not by nature virtuous but all have the potential to become so
through the right effort. That effort includes what today we would call
self-monitoring, the ongoing practice of noting our thoughts and acts.
Other
Greco-Roman philosophic schools used similar practices in their own paths
toward flourishing. For the Stoics, one key was seeing that our feelings about
life’s events, not those events themselves, determine our happiness; we find
equanimity by distinguishing what we can control in life from what we cannot.
Today that creed finds an echo in the popularized Twelve Step version of theologian
Reinhold Niebuhr’s prayer:
God, grant me
the serenity to accept the things I cannot change,
Courage to
change the things I can,
And wisdom to
know the difference.
chuyenngui
The
classical way to the “wisdom to know the difference” lay in mental training.
These Greek schools saw philosophy as an applied art and taught contemplative
exercises and self-discipline as paths to flourishing. Like their peers to the
East, the Greeks saw that we can cultivate qualities of mind that foster
well-being. The Greek practices for developing virtues were to some extent
taught openly, while others were apparently given only to initiates like
Alexander, who noted that the philosopher’s texts were more fully understood in
the context of these secretive teachings.
In
the Greco-Roman tradition, qualities such as integrity, kindness, patience, and
humility were considered keys to enduring well-being. These Western thinkers
and Asian spiritual traditions alike saw the value in cultivating a virtuous
life via a roughly similar transformation of being. In Buddhism, for example,
the ideal of inner flourishing gets put in terms of bodhi (in Pali and
Sanskrit), a path of self-actualization that nourishes “the very best within
oneself.”20
ARISTOTLE’S
DESCENDANTS
Today’s
psychology uses the term well-being for a version of the Aristotelian meme
flourishing. University of Wisconsin psychologist (and
Richie’s
colleague there) Carol Ryff, drawing on Aristotle among many other thinkers,
posits a model of well-being with six arms:
· Self-acceptance,
being positive about yourself, acknowledging both your best and not-so-good
qualities, and feeling fine about being just as you are. This takes a
nonjudgmental self-awareness.
· Personal growth,
the sense you continue to change and develop toward your full potential—getting
better as time goes on—adopting new ways of seeing or being and making the most
of your talents. “Each of you is perfect the way you are,” Zen master Suzuki
Roshi told his students, adding, “and you can use a little improvement”— neatly
reconciling acceptance with growth.
· Autonomy, independence in thought and deed,
freedom from social pressure, and using your own standards to measure yourself.
This, by the way, applies most strongly in individualistic cultures like
Australia and the United States, as compared with cultures like Japan, where
harmony with one’s group looms larger.
· Mastery, feeling
competent to handle life’s complexities, seizing opportunities as they come
your way, and creating situations that suit your needs and values.
· Satisfying
relationships, with warmth, empathy, and trust, along with mutual concern for
each other and a healthy give-and-take.
· Life purpose,
goals and beliefs that give you a sense of meaning and direction. Some
philosophers argue that true happiness comes as a by-product of meaning and
purpose in life.
Ryff
sees these qualities as a modern version of eudaimonia—Aristotle’s “highest of
all human good,” the realization of your unique potential.21 As we will see in
the chapters that follow, different varieties of meditation seem to cultivate
one or more of these capacities. More immediately, several studies have looked
at how meditation boosted people’s ratings on Ryff’s own measure of well-being.
Fewer
than half of Americans, according to the Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, report feeling a strong sense of purpose in life beyond their jobs
and family obligations.22 That particular aspect of wellbeing may have
significant implications: Viktor Frankl has written about how a sense of
meaning and purpose allowed him and select others to survive years in a Nazi
concentration camp while thousands were dying around them.23 For Frankl,
continuing his work as a psychotherapist with other prisoners in the camp lent
purpose to his life; for another man there, it was having a child who was on
the outside; yet another found purpose in the book he wanted to write.
Frankl’s
sentiment resonates with a finding that after a three-month meditation retreat
(about 540 hours total), those practitioners who had strengthened a sense of
purpose in life during that time also showed a simultaneous increase in the
activity of telomerase in their immune cells, even five months later.24 This
enzyme protects the length of telomeres, the caps at the ends of DNA strands
that reflect how long a cell will live.
It’s
as though the body’s cells were saying, stick around—you’ve got important work
to do. On the other hand, as these researchers note, this finding needs to be
replicated in well-designed studies before we can be more sure.
Also
of interest: eight weeks of a variety of mindfulness seemed to enlarge a region
in the brain stem that correlated with a boost in well-being on Ryff’s test.25
But the study was quite small—just fourteen people—and so, needs to be redone
with a larger group before we can draw more than tentative conclusions.
Similarly,
in a separate study, people practicing a popular form of mindfulness reported
higher levels of well-being and other such benefits up to a year later.26 The
more everyday mindfulness, the greater the subjective boost in well-being.
Again, the numbers in this study were small, and a brain measure—which, as
we’ve said, is far less susceptible to psychological skew than
self-evaluations—would be even more convincing.
So,
while we find the conclusion that meditation enhances well-being an appealing
idea, especially as meditators ourselves, our science side remains skeptical.
Studies
such as these are often cited as “proving” the merits of meditation,
particularly these days, when mindfulness has become the flavor du jour. But
meditation research varies enormously when it comes to scientific
soundness—though when used to promote some brand of meditation, app, or other
contemplative “product,” this inconvenient truth goes missing.
In
the chapters that follow, we’ve used rigorous standards to sort out fluff from
fact. What does science actually tell us about the impacts of meditation?
***
4
The Best W